Milton Erickson - Complete Works.pdf

(11427 KB) Pobierz
Initial Experiments Investigating the Nature of Hypnosis
Initial Experiments Investigating the Nature of Hypnosis
Milton H. Erickson
Reprinted with permission from The American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis , October,
1964, 7, 152-162.
INTRODUCTION
During the 1923-24 formal Seminar on Hypnosis at the University of Wisconsin under
the supervision of Clark L. Hull, the author, then an undergraduate student, reported for
the discussion by the postgraduate students of the psychology department upon his own
many and varied experimental investigative findings during the previous six months of
intensive work and on his current studies. There was much debate, argument and
discussion about the nature of hypnosis, the psychological state it constituted, the
respective roles of the operator and the subject, the values and significances of the
processes employed in induction, the nature of the subjects' responses in developing
trances, the possibility of transcendence of normal capabilities, the nature of regression,
the evocation of previously learned patterns of response whether remote or recent, the
processes involved in individual hypnotic phenomenon and in the maintenance of the
trance state, and above all the identification of the primary figure in the development of
the trance state, be it the operator or the subject. The weekly seminars were scheduled for
two hours each, but usually lasted much longer, and frequently extra meetings were
conducted informally in evenings and on weekends and holidays, with most of the group
in attendance.
No consensus concerning the problems could be reached, as opinions and individual
interpretations varied widely, and this finally led the author to undertake a special
investigative project in October 1923. This special study has remained unpublished,
although it was recorded in full at the time, as were many other studies. One of the
reasons for the decision not to publish at that time was the author's dubiousness
concerning Hull's strong conviction that the operator, through what he said and did to the
subject, was much more important than any inner behavioral processes of the subject.
This was a view Hull carried over into his work at Yale, one instance of which was his
endeavor to establish a “standardized technique” for induction. By this term he meant the
use of the same words, the same length of time, the same tone of voice, etc., which finally
eventuated in an attempt to elicit comparable trance states by playing "induction
phonograph records" without regard for individual differences among subjects, and for
their varying degrees of interest, different motivations, and variations in the capacity to
learn. Hull seemed thus to disregard subjects as persons, putting them on a par with
inanimate laboratory apparatus, despite his awareness of such differences among subjects
that could be demonstrated by tachistoscopic experiments. Even so, Hull did demonstrate
that rigid laboratory procedures could be applied in the study of some hypnotic
phenomena.
1
Recently published papers concerning the realities of hypnosis have led to a rereading
and analysis of the author's notebooks in which numerous unpublished studies were fully
recorded. (Credit for this practice should be given to Dr. Hull, and the author often
wonders what happened to the bookshelves of notebooks which Dr. Hull himself
maintained, full of his own unpublished studies.) The rereading of this material produced
the data upon which this paper is based, permitting this report on experimental
investigations into some of the apparent misunderstandings of hypnosis which are still
variously accepted without careful critical thinking.
EXPERIMENTAL PLAN
As originally planned and executed, this early experiment to secure some of the answers
to the intriguing questions confronting the seminar group was so organized that it did not
involve the use of hypnosis. Rather, it was based upon a consideration of the concepts of
introspection developed by E. B. Titchener, Wilhelm Wundt, W. B. Pillsbury, and others,
and was organized as a direct inquiry into these concepts as a possible initial approach to
a later identification of hypnosis or of some of its phenomena. A central consideration in
the proposed experimental project was suggested by the well-known Biblical saying, “As
a man thinketh in his heart, so is he,” a point made in the seminar discussions by several
of the discussants. Professor Joseph Jastrow, who was then head of the psychology
department, aided and advised the author in his plan of experimentation. Jastrow himself
was only slightly interested in hypnosis, but he was interested in the author as a student.
Hull was not consulted, nor did he know of the experiment until it was completed.
SUBJECT SELECTION
The securing of subjects was relatively easy, since any college population offers a wealth
of volunteers. Two elements of selectivity were employed. All students taking
psychology were excluded. All students who were acquainted with the author were
excluded for the reason that they might know that he was interested in hypnosis. Both
male and female undergraduates were employed, most of them by mere chance being
sophomores. Among them there was a predominance of agricultural, home economics,
engineering, commerce, and liberal art students, with an approximately even distribution
of sex, and of comparable ages.
To these students individually, using prepared typewritten material, a plausible,
somewhat interesting, but definitely superficial explanation was given of the concept of
“introspection.” A comparably carefully worded invitation was extended to each of them
to participate in an experiment; this embraced the idea that the experimenter proposed to
do research consisting of “discovering the processes of thought in thinking through from
beginning to end any specified task.” As an illustrative example, it was pointed out that
people know the alphabet and can recite it fluently. However, the majority of those same
people cannot recite the alphabet backward correctly from Z to A except by a slow
“back-and-forth process of thinking.” To those who promptly demonstrated that they
could recite the alphabet backward easily, a second example was offered, namely, the
2
extreme difficulty that would be encountered in reciting backward the entire nursery
rhyme of, “Mary had a little lamb —.”
It was then explained that a much simpler task was in mind for them to do, and they were
earnestly asked not to do any reading of Titchener's "work on thought processes"
(Titchener's name was repeatedly mentioned to discover any previous awareness of his
work, to emphasize "thought processes," and to distract their attention from the word
"introspection ").
They were individually apprised of the possibility that the task might take from one half
to two hours, and a clock was indicated in full view, running silently, located directly in
front of them on a shelf on the laboratory wall. The experimenter, it was explained,
would sit quietly behind a screen some 12 feet to the rear and would not be visible; he
could be spoken to or questioned if the desire or need arose, but it was preferred that the
task once begun be done in complete silence, so that there would be no distractions or
interferences.
What the subjects did not know or observe was that a mirror was so arranged carelessly
among odds and ends in a jumble of laboratory apparatus so that the author had a full
view of the subjects' faces by means of an obscure peephole concealed by the patterned
design on the screen.
From a typewritten copy each subject was separately given the following instructions:
“You are to seat yourself in this chair comfortably, just looking straight ahead.
With your eyes open you are to imagine that there is a small table standing beside
the right (left in the case of those left-handed) arm of the chair. Your arms are to
be resting comfortably in your lap. On that imaginary small table you will
imagine that there is a large fruit bowl filled with apples, pears, bananas, plums,
oranges, or any other kind of fruit you like, but do not turn your head to look in
that direction. All of this imaginary fruit you can imagine as being in easy reach
of your hand resting in your lap.
Next you are to imagine a table of normal height on the bare floor just in front of
you, just far enough away so that you would have to lean a little forward to place
anything on it.
Now the task to be done is for you to sit in the chair looking straight ahead and
mentally go through the processes step by step and in correct order of thinking at
a mental level only of the task of lifting your hand up from your lap, of reaching
up over the arm of the chair, of feeling elbow and shoulder movements, the lateral
extension of your arm, the slight lowering of your hand, the touching of the fruit,
the feel of the fruit, the selection of any one piece, of closing your fingers on it,
lifting it, sensing its weight, moving your hand with the fruit up, back over the
arm of the chair and then placing it on that imaginary table in front of you. That is
all you have to do, just imagine the whole thing. If your eyes get tired or if you
3
can think your thought processes out more clearly with them shut, just close them.
You should expect to make errors in getting each step in the right order, and you
will have to pause and think back just as you would in trying to think the alphabet
(or the nursery rhyme) backward, and it is only reasonable that you will make
mistakes and have to go back and start over again. Just take your time, and do it
carefully, silently, really noting each of your thought processes. If you wish, I will
reread these instructions, and you may realize that perhaps you might have such a
thought as first picking up an apple and then changing your mind and deciding to
pick up an orange. [All subjects wanted a second reading, some a third.]
Now that the instructions are clear, let's look at the bulletin board on the wall over
there, and when the minute hand of the clock is directly on one of the numerals of
the clock face, we will both take our positions and the experiment will begin.”
THE EXPERIMENTAL RESULTS
There were three general types of results obtained from a total of 63 subjects. These may
be classified for the purpose of discussion into three general categories: none; fright
reactions; and full participation.
Concerning the first category, which included 18 subjects, they became restless,
demanded further repetitions of instructions, and finally declared their total disinterest in
the entire project, declaring that they could not do it, that it did not seem to make any
sense, or simply that they were no longer interested in participating. Engineering and
agriculture students predominated in this group. The author's tentative conclusion was
that such students preferred concrete realities to abstract imagining.
The second category, including 13 students, was much more interesting. They became
frightened even to a state of panic, interrupted the experiment to demand reassurance, and
finally refused to continue. (Unfortunately no personality studies had been done on them,
nor did the author then have enough clinical experience to appraise them as
personalities.)
Their reactions were described variously by them, but usually concerned uncontrollable
and involuntary upward movements of the dominant hand; peculiar numb sensations of
the legs, a feeling of rigidity of the body, and a blurring or closing of the eyes that they
felt they could not control. To all of this they reacted with a frightened feeling, which
alarmed them, this alarm then allowing a freedom of action, which led to an emphatic
demand to be excused. The experimenter accompanied his dismissal of them by
elaborately expressed gratitude for the clarity of their demonstrations of "one of the
aspects of intense mental concentration. " This proved to be a most reassuring
manoeuvre, so much so that three subjects then volunteered to repeat the experiment. The
offers were not accepted, assurance being given that the experimenter was already
satisfied with their contribution.
4
The third group, numbering 32, manifested to varying degrees some remarkably similar
forms of behavior. These may be listed as (1) slow loss of the blink reflex; (2) altered
respiratory rhythm; (3) loss of swallowing reflex; (4) development of ideomotor activity
in the dominant hand; (5) exceedingly slow movement of the hand and arm up and over
the arm of the chair; (6) slow closing of the eyes, usually at some point preceding or
during the ideomotor movement of the hand and arm; (7) groping movements of the
fingers, as if selecting an object at the site of the imaginary fruit bowl; (8) a lifting
movement involved in picking up an object, and a slow leaning forward, seemingly
placing the object upon the imaginary table; and (9) then leaning back in the chair and
continuing to rest quietly .
The experimenter was at a loss as to how to proceed the first time that this succession of
events occurred, which was with the third subject. The first two subjects had rejected the
task. Intense study of the quietly resting subject's face indicated that a deep trance had
been induced. Yet there had been no mention of hypnosis; the author's then naiveté and
inexperience with human behavior in a rigid, circumscribed, experimental situation did
not permit him to grasp the significance of the situation immediately. The entire purpose
had been to study behavior in two presumably different circumscribed situations; in one
of these, designated as a hypnotic situation, the author felt that it was distinctly possible
that the operator was the dominant and effective active figure; and the second,
presumably different form of behavior was characterized by the non-participation of the
operation with the subject as the active person.
The subject passively waited, while the experimenter considered that there had been a
foundation for genuine hypnotic rapport because the original joint participatory activity
concerned in the giving and receiving of instructions, the looking at the bulletin board
while awaiting the minute hand of the clock to reach a numeral, and the separate but joint
taking of respective positions. Acting upon this tentative assumption, and still remaining
behind the screen, he remarked, “I think you have certainly worked on this concentrating
long enough now, so it will be all right if you leave, because I have to stay and write this
up.”
Slowly the subject awakened in the manner characteristic of the hypnotic arousal pattern
of behavior, commented, as he looked at the clock, that the time had seemingly passed
remarkably rapidly, and then departed.
The previous two subjects who had failed were engineering students; this one was an
English major. It was reasoned again that the engineers were more interested in concrete
realities, and that the student of literature was interested in abstractions of thought.
Despite this early significant experimental occurrence with the third subject, and thence
the expectation of similar possibilities in the experimenter's mind thereafter, a total of 31
subjects failed in random order, three of them being among the final five, and the very
last subject was a failure in a fashion similar to the first two subjects.
The 32 subjects who manifested hypnoticlike behavior showed various degrees of what
could be regarded as trance states, and some spontaneously made comments aloud about
5
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin