deficyt funkcji wykonawczych ADHD.pdf

(67 KB) Pobierz
92861833 UNPDF
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association
Impact of Executive Function Deficits and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder (ADHD) on Academic Outcomes in Children
Joseph Biederman, Michael C. Monuteaux, Alysa E. Doyle, Larry J. Seidman, Timothy E. Wilens,
Frances Ferrero, Christie L. Morgan, and Stephen V. Faraone
Massachusetts General Hospital
222) ADHD, as ascertained from pediatric and
psychiatric clinics. The authors defined EFD as at least 2 executive function measures impaired.
Significantly more children and adolescents with ADHD had EFDs than did control participants. ADHD
with EFDs was associated with an increased risk for grade retention and a decrease in academic
achievement relative to (a) ADHD alone, (b) controlled socioeconomic status, (c) learning disabilities,
and (d) IQ. No differences were noted in social functioning or psychiatric comorbidity. Children and
adolescents with ADHD and EFDs were found to be at high risk for significant impairments in academic
functioning. These results support screening children with ADHD for EFDs to prevent academic failure.
259) and without ( n
Among the family of mental processes that comprise neuropsy-
chological functioning is the set of higher cortical abilities referred
to as executive functions (EFs). This construct has been defined as
“the ability to maintain an appropriate problem set for attainment
of future goals” (Welsh & Pennington, 1989, p. 201) and includes
such abilities as components of attention, reasoning, planning,
inhibition, set-shifting, interference control, and working memory
(Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996). EFs are considered to be critically
important for complex human behavior, and their breakdown is
thought to commonly result in behavioral or psychiatric impair-
ment (Goldberg & Seidman, 1991). Studies of Alzheimer’s disease
(Chen, Sultzer, Hinkin, Mahler, & Cummings, 1998) and schizo-
phrenia (Green, 1996), as well as studies of patients undergoing
physical rehabilitation (Cahn-Weiner, Malloy, Boyle, Marran, &
Salloway, 2000; Hanks, Rapport, Millis, & Deshpande, 1999),
have clearly demonstrated significant impairments in functional
outcomes associated with EF deficits (EFDs), supporting the crit-
ical role of EFs for sophisticated human behavior.
An emerging literature has repeatedly documented that children
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) exhibit
EFDs. For example, a recent literature review of 18 studies by
Pennington and Ozonoff (1996) concluded that children with
ADHD consistently exhibit worse performance on certain cogni-
tive and EF measures. Likewise, using a focal neuropsychological
battery aimed at assessing EFDs in children and adolescents with
ADHD, we have shown that as a group, boys with ADHD show
significantly poorer executive functioning relative to control par-
ticipants in referred (Seidman, Biederman, Faraone, Weber, &
Ouellette, 1997) and nonreferred (Seidman, Biederman, Monu-
teaux, Weber, & Faraone, 2000) samples. Other studies have
reached similar conclusions (Barkley, 1997; Douglas, 1972). There
is less research investigating EFDs in girls with ADHD. However,
a growing literature that includes research by our group suggests
that EFDs are also found in girls with ADHD (unpublished data).
Despite these consistent data implicating EFDs in ADHD, very
little is known about the clinical implications of EFDs in children
and adolescents with ADHD. Although impairments on such tests
are assumed to relate to real-world functions, the ecological va-
lidity of impairment on such tests and in ADHD has yet to be
determined. Given the critical importance of EFs for adequate
functioning and considering the poor long-term psychiatric, social,
and academic outcome associated with ADHD (Barkley, Fischer,
Edelbrock, & Smallish, 1990; Biederman et al., 1996; Cantwell,
1985; Edelbrock, Costello, & Kessler, 1984; Faraone et al., 1993;
Greene, Biederman, Faraone, Sienna, & Garcia Jetton, 1997; Hart,
Lahey, Loeber, Applegate, & Frick, 1995), it is important to assess
whether the functional impairment related to ADHD is associated
with ADHD itself, independently of EFDs. One approach to ad-
dress this question is to compare the functional outcomes of
ADHD samples with and without EFDs. If children who have
ADHD with EFDs perform worse compared with children with
ADHD without EFDs, there would be evidence that at least part of
the impairment observed in children who have ADHD is associ-
ated with EFDs.
Like other neuropsychological functions, executive functioning
is usually viewed as a continuously varying trait. Yet, there are
several reasons why a categorical definition of EFDs may be
useful. Such a classification would (a) allow for comparisons of
the prevalence of clinically significant EFD across populations, (b)
encourage the standardization of neuropsychological assessment in
Joseph Biederman, Michael C. Monuteaux, Alysa E. Doyle, Larry J.
Seidman, Timothy E. Wilens, Frances Ferrero, Christie L. Morgan, and
Stephen V. Faraone, Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit, Psychiatry De-
partment, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts.
This work was supported, in part, by United States Public Health
Service (National Institute of Mental Health) Grant R01MH-41314 to
Joseph Biederman.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Joseph
Biederman, Massachusetts General Hospital, Pediatric Psychopharmacol-
ogy Unit, Warren 705, 15 Parkman Street, Boston, MA 02114. E-mail:
jbiederman@partners.org
757
2004, Vol. 72, No. 5, 757–766
0022-006X/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.757
The association between executive function deficits (EFDs) and functional outcomes were examined
among children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Participants were
children and adolescents with ( n
758
BIEDERMAN ET AL.
research, (c) aid in the validation of psychiatric diagnoses, and (d)
provide a useful diagnostic tool for clinicians. Specifically for
ADHD, an EFD classification scheme would provide a useful tool
for assessing the association between EFDs and ADHD.
Using the sample with which we demonstrated group neuropsy-
chological deficits in referred (Seidman et al., 1997) and nonre-
ferred (Seidman et al., 2000) children with ADHD, we aimed to
test the association between EFD and academic and psychosocial
impairments among children with ADHD and control participants
at the individual level. On the basis of the literature, we hypoth-
esized that EFDs would be more prevalent in children with ADHD
relative to control participants and would be associated with im-
pairments in multiple domains of functioning.
relied on the epidemiologic version of the Schedule for Affective Disorder
and Schizophrenia for Children (Orvaschel, 1985). Diagnoses were based
on independent interviews with the mothers and direct interviews of
proband participants, except for children younger than 12 years of age, who
were not directly interviewed. Maternal reports and self-reports were
combined by considering a diagnosis positive if it was endorsed by either
interview. The structured interviews assessed lifetime history of psycho-
pathology. ADHD symptoms, based on DSM–III–R criteria, were those
measured at Year 4 for boys and baseline for girls.
The interviewers–psychometricians had undergraduate degrees in psychol-
ogy; they were trained to high levels of interrater reliability for the assessment
of psychiatric diagnosis by Joseph Biederman. We computed kappa coeffi-
cients of agreement by having experienced, board-certified child and adult
psychiatrists diagnose participants from audiotaped interviews made by the
assessment staff. On the basis of 173 interviews from a mixed pediatric and
adult data set, the median kappa for all diagnoses was .86, and the kappa for
ADHD was .98. In addition, the assessment personnel were blind to proband
diagnosis (ADHD or control) and ascertainment site (psychiatric or pediatric).
All follow-up assessments were made blindly to prior assessments of the same
participants and their family members. Thus, all neuropsychological function
assessments were administered and scored by examiners who were unaware of
all other data on the participants.
A committee of board-certified child and adult psychiatrists resolved all
diagnostic uncertainties. The committee members were blind to the par-
ticipants’ ascertainment group, ascertainment site, all data collected from
other family members, and all nondiagnostic data (e.g., neuropsychological
tests). Diagnoses were considered positive if, on the basis of the interview
results, DSM–III–R criteria were unequivocally met to a clinically mean-
ingful degree. We created categories of disorders for this analysis as
follows: (a) mood disorder included major depression with severe impair-
ment or bipolar disorder; (b) multiple anxiety was defined as two or more
anxiety disorders; (c) speech–language was defined as language disorder or
stuttering; (d) disruptive disorder included conduct disorder, oppositional
defiant disorder, or antisocial personality disorder; and (e) psychoactive
substance use disorder included drug or alcohol abuse or dependence.
Rates of disorders reported here are lifetime prevalence.
Method
Participants
In this analysis, the data from two identically designed case-control
family studies of ADHD were combined. These studies ascertained fami-
lies on the basis of male (Biederman et al., 1992) and female (Biederman
et al., 1999) participants with ( n 140 boys; n 140 girls) and without
( n 120 boys; n 122 girls) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (3rd ed., rev.; DSM–III–R ; American Psychiatric Association,
1987) ADHD, as ascertained from pediatric and psychiatric sources. Par-
ticipants were 6 –17 years of age at the time of ascertainment. Male
participants were brought in again for a 4-year follow-up assessment
(Biederman et al., 1996) in which 128 of the proband participants with
ADHD (91%) and 109 of the control proband participants (91%) partici-
pated. There were no significant differences between those participants
successfully reassessed and those lost to follow-up on psychiatric, cogni-
tive, or functional outcomes (Biederman et al., 1996). Potential participants
were excluded if they had been adopted, their nuclear family was not
available, they had major sensorimotor handicaps (e.g., paralysis, deafness,
blindness, psychosis, autism, inadequate command of the English lan-
guage), or they had a full scale IQ (Wechsler, 1974) that was less than 80.
After a complete description of the study, parents provided written in-
formed consent for their children, and children and adolescents provided
written assent, and the IRB granted approval for this study. For the present
study, we used all proband participants with available neuropsychological
data, which included 121 male proband participants with ADHD (95%),
103 male control participants (94%), 138 female proband participants with
ADHD (99%), and 122 female control participants (100%). The few
participants not assessed were due to time constraints, scheduling prob-
lems, or unwillingness on the part of the participants.
A three-stage ascertainment procedure was used to select the proband
participants for both studies. For participants with ADHD, the first stage
was the patient’s referral. The second stage confirmed the diagnosis of
ADHD by using a telephone questionnaire administered to the mother. The
questionnaire asked about the 14 DSM–III–R symptoms of ADHD and
questions regarding study-exclusion criteria. The third stage confirmed the
diagnosis with face-to-face structured interviews with the mother. Only
patients who received a positive diagnosis at all three stages were included.
For control proband participants, we ascertained participants from referrals
to medical clinics for routine physical examinations. In the second stage,
the control mothers responded to the telephone questionnaire. Eligible
control participants meeting study-entry criteria were recruited for the
study and received the third-stage assessment (structured interview). Only
participants classified as not having ADHD at all three stages were in-
cluded in the control group.
Psychosocial Assessments
Social functionin g was assessed with the Social Adjustment Inventory
for Children and Adolescents (SAICA; Orvaschel & Walsh, 1984), a
semistructured interview schedule administered to the mother that assesses
adaptive functioning. The SAICA consists of 76 items that assess social
difficulties at school and in interactions with peers, siblings, and parents.
There is evidence supporting the validity (Biederman, Faraone, & Chen,
1993; Greene et al., 1996; John, Gammon, Prusoff, & Warner, 1987),
interrater reliability (Greene et al., 1997), and internal consistency (Greene
et al., 1997) of the SAICA. As a measure of overall functioning, we used
the DSM–III–R Global Assessment of Functioning (Orvaschel & Puig-
Antich, 1987), a summary score of each participant’s overall functioning
assigned by the interviewers on the basis of information gathered in the
diagnostic structured interview. Socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed
with the Hollingshead Scale (Hollingshead, 1975).
Cognitive Assessments
Using the methods of Sattler (1988), we estimated full scale IQ from the
vocabulary and block design subtests of Wechsler Intelligence Scales for
Children—Revised (WISC–R; Wechsler, 1974) for participants younger
than 17 years of age and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales—Revised
(Wechsler, 1981) for participants older than 17 years of age. Our inter-
viewers assessed academic achievement with the Arithmetic subtest of the
Wide Range Achievement Test—Revised (WRAT–R; Jastak & Jastak,
1985). Participants from the study of boys with ADHD were administered
Psychiatric Assessments
All diagnostic assessments used structured interviews based upon the
criteria of the DSM–III–R . Psychiatric assessments of proband participants
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION DEFICIT IMPACTS ON ACADEMICS
759
the Gilmore Oral Reading Test (Gilmore & Gilmore, 1968) at the baseline
assessment and the Reading subtest of the WRAT–R (Jastak & Jastak,
1985) at follow-up. Participants from the study of girls with ADHD were
administered the Reading subtest of the WRAT–R (Jastak & Jastak, 1985).
The definition of learning disabilities under Public Law 94 –142 requires a
significant discrepancy between a child’s potential and achievement (Fed-
eral Register, 1977). Recommended by Reynolds (1984), we used a sta-
tistically corrected discrepancy between IQ and achievement to define
learning disability.
vant to ADHD, given that such difficulties are also found in
participants with ADHD, even in the absence of learning dis-
ability (LD; Rucklidge & Tannock, 2002). In addition, by cor-
recting for LD in some analyses, we have concluded that the
impairments associated with poor test performance were not
simply due to comorbid LD in the sample.
6. The Freedom from Distractibility Index (Wechsler, 1974, 1981),
which gauges attention and working memory.
Neuropsychological Assessments
To justify our analytical decision to treat the amalgamation of these
variables as a measure of EFD, we subjected them to a factor analysis. The
first factor attained an eigenvalue of 2.66, whereas the second factor had an
eigenvalue of only 0.26, well below the commonly accepted cutoff of unity
for factor retention. This analysis supports the notion that these variables
are all measuring a single latent construct. Thus, although we recognize
that in general EF is considered to be comprised of several factors, the
subtests from these measures used in our battery measure a single factor,
which supports our analytical approach.
The central theoretical construct guiding our choice of many of the tests
in the battery is that key neuropsychological deficits in ADHD are asso-
ciated with frontal regions or frontal networks, indicating impairment in a
widespread cerebral network underlying attention and EFs. The hypothesis
that the neuropsychological underpinnings of ADHD are characterized by
executive dysfunction was proposed by investigators who recognized sim-
ilarities in clinical presentation between persons with hyperactivity and
adult patients with frontal lobe damage (Mattes, 1980; Shue & Douglas,
1992). EFs are distinct from other mental functions such as sensation,
perception, or memory per se. There is, however, considerable overlap with
domains such as attention, reasoning, and problem solving and with certain
components of learning and memory (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996). Thus,
we chose commonly used clinical neuropsychological tests that assess
components of EFs that are thought to be indirect indices of fronto-striatal
systems and that have been used in the research literature on ADHD. These
components of EFs include (a) vigilance and distractibility, (b) planning
and organization, (c) response inhibition, (d) set shifting and categoriza-
tion, (e) selective attention, (f) visual scanning, and (g) verbal learning.
Thus, the neuropsychological battery we developed was based on the
empirical and clinical literatures on attention, ADHD, and EFs. Although
there is no standard battery of EF measures in the field, we specifically
selected tests that have a long track record of use in both clinical settings
and the research literature and that are consistent with our theoretical
perspective. The tests and variables used are as follows:
Statistical Analysis
In defining EFDs, we were compelled to attend to conceptual and
methodological issues. First, we wanted our definition to be clinically
applicable, such that practitioners could readily apply our algorithm with-
out excessive and cumbersome computation. Second, we recognized that
performance on tests of EF improves with age (Denckla, 1996); thus, our
method needed to take the age of the participants into account.
To address age differences in test scores, we divided the control sample
into four groups on the basis of age: 9 years of age or less ( n 29), 10 –13
years of age ( n 81), 14 –17 years of age ( n 78), and 18 years of age
or above ( n 34). These age categories were chosen to reflect matura-
tional growth and development as well as the distribution of control
participants across age in years. For each EF variable within each age
group, we defined a threshold by using the control data that indicated poor
performance if the score was 1.5 standard deviations from the mean of
normally distributed variables or within the poorest 7 th
percentile of per-
1. The copy organization and delay organization of the Rey–
Osterrieth Complex Figure (Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941; scored
by the Waber–Holmes method), which are meant to test planning
and organization.
formance for nonnormally distributed variables.
We then created binary impairment indicators for the EF variables
within age group for all participants (ADHD and control). Thus, we could
sum the number of variables for which any given participant performed
poorly, on the basis of cutoffs derived from the distribution of his or her
age cohort. We defined a participant to have EFDs if the number of tests
defined as impaired was less than two. Three issues contributed to this
choice of cutoff. First, in our previous report (Doyle, Biederman, Seidman,
Weber, & Faraone, 2000), we found that less than two tests impaired
showed the best discrimination between ADHD and non-ADHD partici-
pants. Second, whereas one impaired test may be due to chance, two or
more impaired tests would be likely to be interpreted as a deficit by a
clinician. Third, we felt that it was inappropriate to place individuals with
two abnormal test scores in the nonimpaired group.
To validate our decision to create a binary measure of EFD, we corre-
lated the factor score derived from the factor analysis described earlier to
the number of tests impaired, as defined earlier. We found a modest sized,
significant correlation ( r .59, p .01), supporting our approach.
After applying our EFD algorithm, we were able to define four groups:
(a) control participants without EFD (control participants EFD, N
196), (b) control participants with EFD (control participants EFD, N
26), (c) ADHD without EFD (ADHD EFD, N 173), and (d) ADHD
with EFD (ADHD EFD, N 86). To provide a meaningful illustration
of our definition of impairment, we present the means of the EF variables
across the four groups stratified by age in years in Table 1.
First, we compared the four groups on demographic factors. To address our
hypothesis regarding the effect of EFDs, we modeled the outcomes as a
function of group status and any confounding variables. Statistical models
2. The total errors score (sum of omission, commission, and late
errors) of the Auditory Continuous Performance Test (Weintraub
& Mesulam, 1985), which is intended to measure auditory sus-
tained attention, vigilance, and impulsivity.
3. Perseverative errors and loss of set of the computerized Wiscon-
sin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Heaton, Chelune, Talley, Kay, &
Curtiss, 1993), which measures reasoning ability, concept for-
mation, and cognitive flexibility.
4. The percentage of words learned (number of words recalled
across all trials divided by total number of words) of the Wide
Range Achievement of Memory and Learning test for children
less than 17 years of age (Adams & Sheslow, 1990) or the
California Verbal Learning Test in children greater than or equal
to 17 years of age (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1987), which
is intended to be an index of left prefrontal systems and a
measure of verbal learning and working memory.
5. The color–word raw score of the Stroop test (Golden, 1978),
which is meant to measure response inhibition. Impairments on
this scale could be due to inhibitory difficulties and/or problems
with reading and rapid naming. We consider rapid naming rele-
760
BIEDERMAN ET AL.
Table 1
Executive Function Measures in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Children and
Controls, Stratified by Executive Function Deficits (EFDs)
Executive function measure
Control EFD Control EFD ADHD EFD ADHD EFD
Ages 6–9 (years)
Stroop color–word
23.6
7.2
13.3
6.4
18.0
4.7
14.3
6.6
WCST perseverative errors
18.4
10.9
22.3
15.6
22.3
13.8
33.2
11.8
WCST failure to maintain set
1.9
1.6
0.5
0.6
1.3
1.5
1.3
0.9
Rey delay organization
6.3
3.8
3.3
4.5
4.8
3.1
3.2
2.4
Rey copy organization
6.5
3.4
4.8
2.8
5.8
3.1
3.7
2.7
Auditory CPT mistakes
9.9
5.2
14.0
9.3
10.7
5.3
12.6
6.1
CVLT–WRAML words learned
0.6
0.1
0.6
0.1
0.5
0.1
0.4
0.2
Freedom from Distractibility Index
23.0
4.2
22.8
7.1
21.1
3.9
14.1
3.1
Ages 10–13 (years)
Stroop color–word
31.9
8.5
25.1
8.9
28.0
6.5
23.2
11.4
WCST perseverative errors
13.7
9.2
19.7
13.9
14.2
9.0
23.0
16.1
WCST failure to maintain set
1.0
1.1
2.3
1.5
1.0
1.1
1.3
1.3
Rey delay organization
7.4
4.2
5.0
3.3
7.2
4.1
3.9
2.6
Rey copy organization
9.3
3.2
5.6
3.5
8.8
3.3
5.4
3.6
Auditory CPT mistakes
4.9
3.3
8.1
4.7
4.8
3.0
9.1
5.1
CVLT–WRAML words learned
0.6
0.1
0.6
0.2
0.6
0.1
0.5
0.2
Freedom from Distractibility Index
23.7
5.3
19.4
5.9
20.9
4.4
16.1
4.6
Ages 14–17 (years)
Stroop color–word
42.2
7.6
44.5
8.8
40.8
9.1
27.3
8.1
WCST perseverative errors
10.5
6.8
22.7
15.7
12.3
8.5
21.4
14.1
WCST failure to maintain set
0.7
1.1
1.4
1.3
0.8
1.0
1.8
1.7
Rey delay organization
9.5
4.0
8.0
5.3
9.9
3.3
6.7
4.2
Rey copy organization
11.0
2.8
10.9
2.9
11.1
2.5
8.4
4.3
Auditory CPT mistakes
2.0
1.7
2.7
2.5
2.7
2.7
5.3
5.6
CVLT–WRAML words learned
0.7
0.1
0.6
0.2
0.7
0.1
0.5
0.1
Freedom from Distractibility Index
23.9
4.5
21.5
6.5
21.0
4.8
15.8
4.2
18 (years)
Stroop color–word
45.1
8.5
38.5
15.2
43.3
8.5
34.2
8.5
WCST perseverative errors
6.8
4.0
15.8
10.2
7.5
3.2
18.5
8.5
WCST failure to maintain set
0.4
0.9
2.3
3.2
0.9
1.2
1.9
1.2
Rey delay organization
10.5
3.6
11.0
2.3
10.1
3.6
7.6
4.1
Rey copy organization
12.1
1.8
10.3
3.4
10.8
3.3
8.0
3.0
Auditory CPT mistakes
1.2
1.2
2.3
2.6
1.5
1.8
4.4
3.3
CVLT–WRAML words learned
0.7
0.1
0.6
0.0
0.7
0.1
0.6
0.2
Freedom from Distractibility Index
24.4
5.6
21.8
2.6
23.4
4.3
15.1
4.6
Note. Values in the table represent means plus or minus the standard deviations. WCST
Wisconsin Card
Sorting Test; CPT
Continuous Performance Test; CVLT
California Verbal Learning Test; WRAML
Wide Range Achievement of Memory and Learning.
were fit with the statistical software package STATA (Stata Corporation,
1997). We used generalized estimating equation models with the appropriate
link and family specification depending on the distribution of the outcome
variable (i.e., binary or continuous). All statistical tests were two-tailed. The
statistical significance of each covariate in these regression models was deter-
mined by Wald’s test. To assess normality, we used the Shapiro–Wilk test. We
used an alpha level of .05 to assert statistical significance.
SES score (indicating higher social class status) as compared with
the ADHD EFD group. No differences were noted in gender
across the four groups, and the two ADHD groups did not differ in
the rate of current medication status. Because the key comparison
was between the two ADHD groups and the age difference noted
earlier between the control EFD and ADHD EFD groups was
not substantial from a developmental perspective, all subsequent
analyses were statistically adjusted for SES but not for years of age.
Results
We found that 86 (33%) of the participants with ADHD were
classified as having EFDs, whereas only 26 (12%) of the control
participants were classified as having EFDs,
Clinical Features of ADHD
(1, N 481)
30.9, p .01. This association between ADHD and EFD remained
after statistical adjustment for gender, age, IQ, LD, and SES. As
shown in Table 2, there were small but statistically significant
differences across the groups in years of age. Control partici-
pants EFD were on average 1.3 years older than ADHD EFD
participants. In addition, children and adolescents without EFDs
(ADHD and control participants) had a significantly lower mean
2
We first investigated whether EFD was associated with the
clinical features of ADHD. There were no differences between
proband participants with ADHD with and without EFDs in the
age at onset of ADHD (3.1 2.2 vs. 3.2 2.4, respectively),
t (257) 0.59, p .55. Only 2 of 14 DSM–III–R symptoms were
more common in ADHD EFD proband participants relative to
ADHD EFD proband participants. There were no differences
between proband participants with ADHD with and without EFDs
Ages
92861833.001.png
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION DEFICIT IMPACTS ON ACADEMICS
761
Table 2
Demographic Characteristics in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Children and Controls, Stratified by Executive
Functioning Deficits (EFDs)
Control EFD
( N 196)
Control EFD
( N 26)
ADHD EFD
( N 173)
ADHD EFD
( N 86)
Omnibus analyses
Demographic feature
MSD n (%)
MDn (%)
MSD n (%)
MDn (%)
df
F
p
Age (years)
13.7 3.7 a *
13.4 4.4
13.1 3.5
12.3 3.7
3, 477
3.3
.019
SES
1.6 0.8 a **
1.7 0.9
1.7 0.8 a **
2.2 1.1
3, 476
11.1
.001
df
2
p
Gender
Girls
106 (54)
16 (62)
91 (53)
47 (55)
3
0.8
.862
Boys
90 (46)
10 (38)
82 (47)
39 (45)
Medication status
0 (0)
0 (0)
104 (60)
51 (59)
3
196.0
.001
Note. SES
socioecomonic status; Medication status
any psychotropic medication at the time of neuropsychological assessment.
a Versus ADHD
EFD.
* p
.05. ** p
.01.
on the number of hyperactive–impulsive symptoms (6.2 1.7 vs.
6.0 1.7, respectively), t (258) 0.89, p .38, or total symptoms
(11.1 2.7 vs. 10.2 3.4, respectively), t (258) 1.87, p .06.
However, there were more inattentive symptoms among ADHD
EFD children and adolescents compared with ADHD EFD
children and adolescents (5.6 0.7 vs. 5.2 1.1, respectively),
t (258) 2.79, p .01.
continuous measure of EF showed that poorer EF functioning
significantly predicted worsening academic performance as mea-
sured by repeating a grade, LD, IQ, WRAT–R arithmetic, and
WRAT–R reading.
It is possible that, because of our use of two measures from the
same test, namely the copy and delay organization score from the
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure and the perseverative errors and
failure to maintain set score from the WCST, participants were
designated as having EFD on the basis of a single test. This gave
these two tests more influence over the EFD measure than the
other tests. To account for this potential problem, we recalculated
our EFD measure, excluding any participants who were designated
as EFD only because of deficits on the two Rey–Osterrieth Com-
plex Figure variables or the two WCST variables. Only 7 partic-
ipants (2 from the control group and 5 from the group with ADHD)
were dropped from the analysis because of this problem. We
repeated the analysis of the academic functioning outcomes with-
out these 7 participants, and the results did not change.
EFDs and Academic Functioning
As shown in Table 3, there were several differences across
groups in achievement and school functioning. Children and ado-
lescents with ADHD with and without EFDs performed worse than
control participants on achievement scores and measures of school
functioning, and ADHD EFD children and adolescents demon-
strated significantly poorer performance on every academic out-
come assessed relative to the ADHD EFD group. In contrast,
school performance did not differ meaningfully in control partic-
ipants irrespective of the presence or absence of EFDs.
To further test the effect of EFDs within ADHD, we ran addi-
tional analyses on the academic outcomes including the partici-
pants with ADHD only. We found that ADHD EFD participants
were over 2 times more likely to repeat a grade compared with
ADHD EFD participants, even after controlling for SES, LD,
and IQ. Children and adolescents with ADHD EFD were almost
3 times more likely to have a LD relative to ADHD EFD
children and adolescents, controlling for SES and IQ. In addition,
among children and adolescents with ADHD, EFDs were associ-
ated with a statistically significant average decrease of over 10
points on the IQ score, controlling for LD and SES, and 4 points
on each WRAT–R score, controlling for SES, LD, and IQ. To
further show the robustness of the effect of EFDs among children
and adolescents with ADHD, we repeated these analyses using a
continuous measure of EF. We standardized the EF measures
(within age strata) and created a linear combination by summing
over these z scores. We then standardized this sum and used the
resulting z score as an independent variable in models predicting
the academic outcomes, with the same statistical adjustments used
earlier. As in the analysis that used a binary measure of EFD, the
EFDs and Social and Psychiatric Outcomes
Table 4 shows the social and psychiatric outcomes in children
with ADHD and in control participants, stratified by EFDs. Al-
though participants with ADHD had significantly more impaired
performance on global functioning (Global Assessment of Func-
tioning scores) and interpersonal functioning (SAICA total scores)
than control participants, these differences were not associated
with EFDs. Similar patterns emerged for findings of psychiatric
comorbidity: Proband participants with ADHD had higher rates of
comorbid disruptive mood and anxiety disorders than control
participants, irrespective of the presence or absence of EFDs, and
no differences were identified in comparisons within ADHD and
control participants with and without EFDs.
Effect of Development on EFDs and Functional Outcomes
We tested whether the association between EFDs and academic,
social, and psychiatric outcomes is influenced by neuropsycholog-
ical development across childhood. We used age as a proxy for
92861833.002.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin