neuropsychologiczne korelaty symptomów adhd.pdf

(126 KB) Pobierz
92861840 UNPDF
2005, Vol. 19, No. 4, 446–455
Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association
Neuropsychological Correlates of ADHD Symptoms in Preschoolers
David J. Marks
Mount Sinai Medical Center
Olga G. Berwid, Amita Santra, Elizabeth C. Kera,
and Shana E. Cyrulnik
Graduate Center of the City University of New York
Jeffrey M. Halperin
Mount Sinai Medical Center, Graduate Center of the City University of New York, and Queens College of the City
University of New York
The authors examined the neuropsychological status of 22 preschoolers at risk for attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and 50 matched control children, using measures of nonverbal working
memory, perceptual and motor inhibition, and memory for relative time. All tasks included paired control
conditions, which allowed for the isolation of discrete executive function constructs. Group differences
were evident on several measures of neuropsychological functioning; however, after accounting for
nonexecutive abilities, no deficits could be attributed to specific functions targeted by the tasks.
Performance on executive measures was not related to objective indices of activity level or ratings of
ADHD symptoms. Yet, the fact that at-risk preschoolers were highly symptomatic casts doubt on whether
executive function deficits and/or frontostriatal networks contribute etiologically to early behavioral
manifestations of ADHD.
Keywords: ADHD, preschool children, executive function
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a clinically
heterogeneous syndrome characterized by symptoms of inatten-
tion, hyperactivity, and impulsivity that are developmentally inap-
propriate and negatively impact an individual’s psychosocial func-
tioning. Research exploring the biological underpinnings of
ADHD has focused on a host of etiologic mechanisms, and al-
though the specific nature of the dysfunction remains unknown,
data from the fields of neuropsychology (Barkley, 1994, 1997;
Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996), neurochemistry (Solanto, 1998),
neuroimaging (Hynd et al., 1993), and molecular genetics (Rowe
et al., 1998) suggest that the core behavioral features of the
disorder and associated neurocognitive deficits likely reflect a
multifactorial etiologic process involving circuits of the prefrontal
cortex.
Several investigators have suggested that the cognitive sequelae
of ADHD primarily encompass deficits in executive functioning.
Although many definitions have been proposed (Eslinger, 1996),
executive functions are believed to incorporate planning, self-
regulation, organizational skills, goal setting, problem solving, and
judgment (Lezak, 1993) and are thought to be mediated by recip-
rocal connections between the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia.
Consistent with such models, school-age children with the disorder
have been shown to perform significantly worse than control
children on a host of neuropsychological indices believed to be
subserved by frontal–subcortical pathways, including measures of
working memory (Barkley, Grodzinsky, & DuPaul, 1992), plan-
ning (Pennington, Grossier, & Welsch, 1993), cognitive flexibility
(Chelune, Fergusson, Koon, & Dickey, 1986; Tripp, Ryan, &
Peace, 2002), time perception (Rubia, Taylor, Taylor, & Sergeant,
1999), motor inhibition (Oosterlaan & Sergeant, 1985; Trommer,
Hoeppner, Lorber, & Armstrong, 1988), and phonemic fluency
(Grodzinsky & Barkley, 1999).
While some investigators have gone so far as to suggest that the
symptoms of ADHD and related information-processing distur-
bances result from a primary deficit in frontostriatal executive
functions, other researchers have been unable to provide empirical
support for such claims (Loge, Staton, & Beatty, 1990) or have
argued that executive theories of ADHD should be interpreted
cautiously because of a host of methodological limitations (e.g.,
small sample sizes, variability in selection and/or diagnostic cri-
teria), small effect sizes (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996), and, in
some cases, disregard for psychiatric comorbidity (Sergeant,
Geurts, & Oosterlaan, 2002). Other researchers (e.g., Tripp et al.,
2002) have suggested that the lack of behavioral control observed
in children with ADHD might adversely affect their test-taking
skills, irrespective of the constructs under investigation, which in
turn could contribute to deficits in nonexecutive domains (e.g.,
language, spatial abilities, etc.). Moreover, the fact that ADHD
probands, even after controlling for psychiatric comorbidity (Fara-
one, Biederman, Weber, & Russell, 1998), typically perform more
poorly than matched controls on measures of cognitive functioning
(Shallice et al., 2002) raises the possibility that intellectual com-
promise, not executive dysfunction per se, accounts for group
David J. Marks, Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Mount
Sinai Medical Center. Olga G. Berwid, Amita Santra, Elizabeth C. Kera,
and Shana E. Cyrulnik, Neuropsychology Doctoral Program, Graduate
Center of the City University of New York. Jeffrey M. Halperin, Division
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Mount Sinai Medical Center; Neuro-
psychology Doctoral Program, Graduate Center of the City University of
New York; Department of Psychology, Queens College of the City Uni-
versity of New York.
This research was supported by Professional Staff Congress–City Uni-
versity of New York Grant 6269500-31 awarded to Jeffrey M. Halperin.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to David J.
Marks, Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Box 1230, Mount
Sinai Medical Center, 1 Gustave L. Levy Place, New York, NY 10029.
E-mail: david.marks@mssm.edu
446
Neuropsychology
0894-4105/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.446
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF PRESCHOOL ADHD
447
differences on neuropsychological indices. By extension, control-
ling for intellectual abilities has been shown to eradicate or atten-
uate group disparities in neuropsychological status (Seidman,
Biederman, Faraone, Weber, & Ouellette, 1997; Tripp et al.,
2002).
Although studies of school-age children are replete with explo-
rations into the neurobiological and neurocognitive underpinnings
of ADHD, such efforts have been far more limited with samples of
preschool children (Baving, Laucht, & Schmidt, 1999; Mariani &
Barkley, 1997). Several factors may account for this dichotomy.
First, neuroimaging techniques are difficult to use with preschool
children because of the confounding effects of movement artifact,
and therefore, relative to other forms of data, may be less able to
tap into functional/neurocognitive impairments. Further, among
the measures used to evaluate information-processing models or
executive dysfunction theories, many are inappropriate for use
with preschoolers because they are often too long, insufficiently
engaging, or require reading skills that have not yet developed.
Within the past decade, a number of researchers (e.g., Corkum,
Byrne, & Ellsworth, 1995; Espy, Kaufmann, McDiarmid, &
Glisky, 1999) have responded to this perceived deficiency by
developing a host of tasks appropriate for use with preschool
children. In large part, such investigations have demonstrated that
the preschool years are characterized by rapid cognitive growth
typified by the emergence and maturation of elementary abstrac-
tion, aspects of cognitive flexibility (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001),
response inhibition, basic set-switching skills (Espy, 1997), rule
use, working memory (Espy et al., 1999), visual search (Welsch,
Pennington, & Grossier, 1991), motor inhibition, impulse control,
planning, and attention regulation (Klenberg, Korkman, & Lahti-
Nuuttila, 2001). A subset of tasks designed to assess these con-
structs may be most appropriately conceptualized as measures
used to assess the core behavioral features of ADHD (e.g., atten-
tional deficits and overactivity), whereas others have been adapted
from animal models and studies of human adults to specifically
assess domains of neuropsychological functioning (e.g., working
memory, inhibitory control, set shifting).
Byrne, DeWolfe, and Bawden (1998) reported that, relative to
matched controls, preschoolers with ADHD committed a signifi-
cantly greater number of commission errors on a selective attention
task and were comparatively unable to restrain themselves from
touching a series of off limits toys (77% of probands vs. 0% of
controls). In contrast, they found no group difference in the num-
ber of cancellation task omission errors and did not identify dis-
crepancies in ratings of off-task behavior obtained during the
structured assessment procedure. Contrary to the nearly ubiquitous
reports of hyperactivity communicated by parents of ADHD chil-
dren during the interview process, both groups evinced similar
levels of out-of-seat behavior during structured and unstructured
periods and were indistinguishable with regard to their degree of
mobility while engaged in unstructured play. However, a subse-
quent study by the same investigators (DeWolfe, Byrne, & Baw-
den, 2000) using a larger cohort of preschool children did not
replicate many of their earlier results. Specifically, no significant
group differences emerged in objectively defined levels of impul-
sivity (rates of unsanctioned play: 60% of probands vs. 36% of
controls), whereas robust discrepancies were reported for measures
of off-task behavior and observed mobility during a play period
(ADHD controls).
As to the question of whether preschoolers with ADHD
display neuropsychological deficits that mirror their school-age
counterparts, the answer has yet to be conclusively determined.
Relative to matched controls, preschool boys with ADHD have
been shown to exhibit deficits in motor control and working
memory (Mariani & Barkley, 1997) and impairments in aspects
of academic achievement. Hughes, Dunn, and White (1998)
reported significant group differences between hard-to-manage
preschoolers and control preschoolers on four of six executive
function indices, including measures of inhibitory control,
working memory, and cognitive flexibility. However, virtually
all group differences were no longer significant after controlling
for verbal ability and socioeconomic status. In addition, the fact
that performance on such tasks was also highly associated with
acts of aggression and antisocial behavior (Hughes, White,
Sharpen, & Dunn, 2000) calls into question whether patterns of
executive dysfunction were related to features of ADHD per se.
Sonuga-Barke, Dalen, Daley, and Remington (2002), after par-
tialing out the effects of age and IQ, did not find an association
between working memory or planning skills and symptoms of
ADHD in a heterogeneous sample of preschool children.
Rather, the authors proposed that compromised behavioral in-
hibition may serve as a harbinger for the emergence of more
global executive function deficits that appear later in
development.
Taken together, the findings from the above studies provide
limited support for the notion that preschool children with mani-
festations of ADHD display unique patterns of neurocognitive
functioning relative to normally developing preschool youths. Un-
fortunately, such findings have been inconsistent because of vari-
ations in selection criteria, assessment techniques, and statistical
methodology (e.g., covariation for age and/or IQ), which in turn,
render it difficult to draw precise conclusions regarding both the
concurrent and predictive utility of neuropsychological parameters
in disruptive preschoolers. Furthermore, none of the tasks used in
previous studies incorporated paired reference conditions to isolate
the construct or constructs of interest. Consequently, it is unclear
whether reports of executive dysfunction reflect genuine deficits in
specific neurocognitive domains (e.g., visual working memory) or
whether such phenomena occur secondary to problems with basic-
level functions (e.g., visual form discrimination) subsumed by
executive function indices.
The current study sought to remedy this gap by using four
novel executive function tasks designed to assess the following:
(a) memory for time and temporal sequencing, (b) nonverbal
working memory, (c) perceptual inhibition (i.e., susceptibility
to interference), and (d) motor inhibition (i.e., inhibition of
prepotent responding). All neuropsychological indices con-
sisted of both experimental and control conditions to isolate
process specificity and have been designed to be both brief and
engaging. Key issues addressed by this investigation included
(a) the extent to which at-risk and typically developing pre-
school children differ on the above executive function param-
eters, (b) the relationship of laboratory measures of activity
level to performance on the executive function tasks, and (c) the
association between early neuropsychological status and parent
and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms.
448
MARKS ET AL.
Method
group (51 boys, 51 girls). The ratio of boys to girls among at-risk partic-
ipants (3:1) significantly exceeded that of controls (1:1),
2 (1, N
Participants
.002. Among the 102 children from the Phase 1 cohort
who met criteria for the control group, 50 participants (23 boys, 27 girls)
agreed to come to the laboratory at Queens College for further on-site
assessments. Control participants who were and were not seen for Phase 2
evaluations did not differ significantly with regard to parent and teacher
ratings of ADHD symptom domains (all p s .05). Among the 56 children
who met criteria for at-risk status, 22 (18 boys, 4 girls) were seen for
Phase 2 assessments. Once again, at-risk participants who did and did not
complete the Phase 2 evaluations did not differ significantly with respect to
dimensional ratings of inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity completed
by parents and teachers (all p s
9.33, p
Recruitment was conducted using a two-step procedure, previously
described by Campbell, Ewing, Breaux, and Szumowski (1986), to gather
parent and teacher ratings on a relatively large sample of children from
which an assessment cohort could then be selected. During this segment of
the project (Phase 1), principals of private and public preschools located in
Queens, New York, were contacted and permission was requested to screen
the school for children with attention and behavior problems. A subset of
Phase 1 participants was subsequently selected for inclusion in the on-
campus assessment (Phase 2) portion of the study. Selection procedures for
both project components are outlined below.
Phase 1 screening procedure. Thirteen preschools within close prox-
imity to Queens College agreed to participate, and parents of children in
these schools were sent Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (4th ed.; DSM–IV ; American Psychiatric Association, 1994)
ADHD checklists along with consent forms that would allow us to collect
similar ratings from the child’s teacher. The checklists consisted of the 18
ADHD behaviors listed in DSM–IV , which were rated on a 4-point scale
(0
.05).
As shown in Table 1, the two groups that came to the laboratory (i.e., at
risk and control) did not differ significantly in age or estimated intellectual
functioning (according to the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of
Intelligence—Revised Information subtest, Wechsler, 1989). However, the
at-risk group included disproportionately more boys relative to the control
group (81% vs. 46%),
.01. Significant
differences were also observed with regard to dimensional ratings of
inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity symptoms reported by parents
and teachers, which is not surprising given that DSM–IV ADHD checklist
ratings were used for group classification. However, the groups also
differed significantly in activity level as measured using solid-state acti-
graphs, providing further support for their validity (see Table 1). Dispar-
ities in objectively assessed activity level occurred in the absence of
differences in actigraph variability over time (all p s
2 (1, N
72)
7.35, p
very much ). As
has been suggested by other investigators (e.g., DuPaul et al., 1998;
Kadesj¨ , Kadesj¨, H¨ggl¨f, & Gillberg, 2001), a symptom was considered
to be present if it received a rating of 2 ( pretty much )or3( very much ). All
consent forms and behavioral rating scales were returned directly to the
principal investigator to preserve both the confidentiality of parent reports
and the objectivity of teacher ratings.
As a result of these mailings, we received consent and both parent and
teacher ratings for 212 children (123 boys, 89 girls). Children for whom
DSM–IV ADHD checklists were completed ranged in age from 2.84 to 5.92
years with a mean age of 4.38 (S D
not at all ,1
s omewhat, 2
pretty much, and 3
.05; see Table 1). Of
note, separate consent forms were used for each portion (i.e., Phases 1 and
2) of the study, and parents were informed during the screening phase and
subsequent invitational telephone contact that participation in Phase 1 did
not obligate them to participate in the on-campus assessment.
Although racial/ethnic data were not collected on the screening form,
among the children evaluated in the laboratory, 35% were Caucasian, 3%
were African American, 21% were Latino, 24% were Asian American, and
18% were of other or mixed ethnicity according to demographic data
obtained from the parents of all Phase 2 participants. The families who
participated in the on-campus evaluation were of primarily middle-class
status, with 47% reporting a family income greater than $70,000 per year,
and 90% of the families reporting a total income above $25,000 per year.
Additionally, 44% of fathers and 48% of mothers reportedly had received
4-year undergraduate degrees. Among the children, 86% lived with both
parents, who were married. Parents were compensated $10.00 for trans-
0.63) years.
Selection of groups for Phase 2. Designation of at-risk and control
status was based on combined parent–teacher ratings modeled after
DSM–IV ADHD criteria. To be considered at risk, a child needed to receive
a pretty much or very much rating on at least six inattention or hyperactive–
impulsive items by either informant. To meet criteria for the control group,
a child needed to receive a pretty much or very much rating on fewer than
three inattention and three hyperactive–impulsive items according to both
parent and teacher ratings. This more liberal method of classification of
at-risk status was used to cast a wide net for children with behavioral
difficulties.
On the basis of these criteria, 56 (25.3%) children (42 boys, 14 girls) met
criteria for the at-risk group, and 102 (46.2%) met criteria for the control
Table 1
Phase 2 Sample Characteristics
Control ( N 50)
At risk ( N 22)
Variable
M
SD
M
SD
t or F
p
Age
4.23
0.69
4.46
0.46
1.43
.10
WPPSI-R information
10.50
3.44
8.95
3.00
1.65
.10
Hyperactivity–impulsivity total: Parent
4.66
3.05
16.95
5.33
10.11
.001
Inattention total: Parent
4.74
3.05
10.68
6.05
4.37
.001
Hyperactivity–impulsivity total: Teacher
2.70
4.59
12.68
10.26
4.38
.001
Inattention total: Teacher
2.56
3.61
10.36
8.21
4.28
.001
Actigraph: Ankle a
655.03
117.24
1,175.77
196.79
5.12
.05
Actigraph: Waist a
220.87
37.90
381.41
63.61
4.66
.05
Actigraph variability: Ankle b
2,269.46
227.34
2,865.16
381.61
1.78
.10
Actigraph variability: Waist b
681.31
61.43
868.81
103.12
2.42
.10
Note. WPPSI-R Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Revised.
a Data reflect age-controlled median and standard error values, respectively.
b Age-controlled data correspond to average intraindividual dispersion around
actigraph values and standard error values, respectively.
158)
92861840.001.png
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL CORRELATES OF PRESCHOOL ADHD
449
portation costs associated with the campus visit; children were provided
with snacks and stickers during the assessment.
There were no gender or ethnic restrictions, but both the children and
their parents were required to be English speaking. Children diagnosed
with mental retardation, a pervasive developmental disorder, or a neuro-
logic disorder (e.g., epilepsy) and those who were taking systemic medi-
cation for a chronic medical condition were excluded from participation.
3. Executive function battery. The Perceptual and Motor Conflict Test
(Nassauer & Halperin, 2003) consists of separate perceptual (stimulus) and
motor (response) conflict components, both of which required the child to
be seated in front of a computer monitor with an oversized two-button
computer mouse.
The SCT required fewer than 5 min to administer and consisted of three
conditions. For Condition 1, a series of left- or right-pointing arrows
appeared in the center of the screen. The child was required to press the
button corresponding to the side to which the arrow was pointing (direc-
tion). For Condition 2, a series of rectangular boxes appeared on either the
left or right side of the monitor, and the child pressed the button on the side
on which the rectangle appeared (location). Condition 3 consisted of trials
in which either left- or right-pointing arrows appeared on either the left or
right side of the screen. Children were required to inhibit the prepotent
response to stimulus location and to respond instead to arrow direction.
Errors and reaction times (RTs) for Condition 3 were shown to be signif-
icantly greater than for either of the other two conditions (Nassauer &
Halperin, 2003). For all three conditions, stimuli remained on the screen
until the child responded, with a subsequent interstimulus interval of 1,000
ms prior to the onset of the next stimulus. This task assessed the construct
of interference control similar to that of the Stroop Color and Word Test
(Golden, 1978). However, unlike the Stroop test, it can be used with young
children because it does not require reading.
The RCT is a brief ( 5 min) computerized measure of response orga-
nization and motor inhibition. The first (compatible) condition was iden-
tical to that of the SCT; children were asked to press a button that
corresponded to the direction that a centrally located arrow was pointing.
Following 20 trials, the task changed such that the child was asked to press
the button that was on the opposite side from where the arrow was pointing
(incompatible condition). The child was therefore required to inhibit a
prepotent response and engage a new response set. Data indicated signif-
icant differences in error rates and RTs between compatible and incom-
patible task conditions (Nassauer & Halperin, 2003). As in the SCT, stimuli
remained on the screen until the child responded, with a subsequent
interstimulus interval of 1,000 ms prior to the onset of the next stimulus.
The DNMST is a nonverbal memory task that required the children to
hold a visual image online and is analogous to tasks used with monkeys to
demonstrate deficits following prefrontal cortex lesions (Goldman, Ros-
vold, Vest, & Galkin, 1971). Nonverbalizable figural stimuli were pre-
sented to the children on a computer screen for 4 s, followed by a 1-s delay.
Then, a response screen containing the original figure and one new figure
was presented. The children were required to identify the new figure by
pointing to it. This task’s difficulty level increased every three trials, such
that the first three trials contained stimuli with a single figure and a
response screen containing two figures, the second level contained two
figural stimuli and three response options, and so forth. The control task
was a nonmatching-to-sample task with no delay: The children viewed the
stimulus and response figures simultaneously. Both task conditions were
administered until a child got two or three trials incorrect within a given
level (up to four task levels; possible score range 0–12).
The RMT was designed to measure memory for relative time and
temporal sequencing and has been shown to effectively distinguish patients
with prefrontal cortex lesions from those with lesions in other parts of the
brain (Milner & Petrides, 1984). Verbalizable pictorial stimuli were pre-
sented on a computer screen at a rate of one per second. After the pictured
stimuli were displayed, a response screen containing two pictures was
presented. In the control (recognition) condition, only one response picture
had been previously seen, and the child was asked to indicate, by pointing,
which picture had been seen before. In the memory-for-time condition,
both pictures on the response screen had been previously seen, and the
children indicated which picture had been seen last, or most recently.
Similar to the DNMST, conditions increased in difficulty every three trials;
Procedures
The on-campus assessment took approximately 2 hr to complete and
involved the gathering of demographic information from the parent and
testing data from the child. Three forms of data were collected from the
children: (a) objective measures of activity level to validate the distinction
between at-risk and control groups, (b) an estimate of overall cognitive
function to characterize the sample, and (c) measures to assess four aspects
of executive functioning, including inhibition of response to distracting
stimuli (Stimulus Conflict Task [SCT]), response organization (Response
Conflict Task [RCT]), nonverbal working memory (Delayed Nonmatching
to Sample Test [DNMST]), and memory for time (Recency Memory Test
[RMT]). In light of reports highlighting the importance of process speci-
ficity (Sergeant et al., 2002), all neuropsychological indices were devel-
oped in our laboratory and consisted of both experimental and control/
reference conditions that would allow for the isolation of the constructs of
interest. All participants were administered practice trials (on paper and
computer) prior to the start of each task to ensure comprehension of the
relevant instructions. Practice trials were repeated until participants could
demonstrate a sufficient level of proficiency, operationalized as two or
three correct out of three practice trials for the DNMST and RMT. More
subjective criteria were used to gauge comprehension of SCT and RCT
instructions. A fixed test-administration order was used to systematically
vary task types to help maintain interest and limit fatigue effects.
1. Objective measures of activity level. Motor activity was recorded
throughout the Phase 2 assessment using two solid-state actigraphs worn
around the waist and nondominant ankle. Both actigraphs (Model WAM-
7164) were obtained from Computer Sciences and Applications, Inc., of
Shalimar, Florida, and utilize internal accelerometers (acceleration magni-
tude, 0.05–2.00 G; frequency response, 0.25–2.50 Hz) to store data on the
number of movements per unit time. Prior to each Phase 2 assessment, both
actigraphs were initialized using a reader interface unit and programmed to
obtain movement counts in 60-s epochs. After each evaluation, the acti-
graphs were returned to the reader interface unit, and the data were
downloaded into Microsoft Excel spreadsheets. Descriptive statistics (i.e.,
mean, median, and standard deviation) were generated separately for each
participant’s ankle and waist actigraph data, using computational algo-
rithms included within Microsoft Excel. The same two actigraphs were
used during all Phase 2 assessments and were consistently placed on the
same location on each child (i.e., waist and nondominant ankle).
It is important to note that assessments of activity level taken during
structured test sessions are reliable (Reichenbach, Halperin, Sharma, &
Newcorn, 1992), yield measures that are correlated with parent and teacher
ratings of hyperactivity (Fairweather, Reilly, Grant, Whittaker, & Paton,
1999; Reichenbach et al., 1992), and have been shown to effectively
discriminate hard-to-manage preschool boys from comparison controls
(Campbell, Pierce, March, Ewing, & Szumowski, 1994). In addition,
Fairweather et al. (1999) have shown Computer Sciences and Applications
actigraphs to be a valid method for assessing physical activity in preschool
children and have identified significant associations between actigraph
readings obtained from different locations on the torso (i.e., left vs. right
hip).
2. General cognitive functioning: Wechsler Preschool and Primary
Scale of Intelligence—Revised Information subtest (Wechsler, 1989).
This well-normed subtest has a strong association with general intelligence
and was administered to characterize the sample and to identify possible
group differences with regard to general cognitive ability.
450
MARKS ET AL.
administration was discontinued if a child provided two incorrect responses
out of three within a task level (up to eight task levels; possible score range
for each condition
RCT
0–24).
As predicted, a significant main effect for condition was
identified, such that participants, regardless of group, commit-
ted significantly more errors on the incompatible condition
relative to the compatible condition, F (1, 59) 14.41, p
.001, 2
Data Analysis
Objective 1: Profiles of neuropsychological status in at-risk versus
control preschoolers. Data from each of the executive function tasks
were submitted to a Group (control vs. at risk) Condition (control vs.
experimental) mixed factorial analysis of variance. Using this approach, a
main effect for condition, reflecting superior performance in the control
condition, would support the validity of the task manipulation. A main
effect for group, with those in the at-risk group performing more poorly,
would suggest greater cognitive impairment in that group. Given that
control and experimental task conditions were identical except for the
degree of executive function involvement, as posited by the additive factors
model (Sergeant et al., 2002), a deficit specific to a given executive
function would be reflected by significant a Group
.20 (see Figure 1A). In addition, a significant main
effect for group status was also observed, with at-risk partici-
pants committing significantly more errors across both RCT
conditions, F (1, 59) 29.53, p .001, 2
.33. However, as
evidenced by the parallel slopes, at-risk preschoolers did not
perform differentially worse on the incompatible condition rel-
ative to the compatible condition, F (1, 59) 0.40, p .10, 2
.007.
A similar pattern emerged when RT was used as the dependent
measure. Specifically, both groups evinced longer RTs for the
incompatible condition relative to the compatible condition: con-
dition main effect, F (1, 59) 12.94, p .001, 2
Condition interaction.
RTs and error rates served as dependent measures for the RCT and SCT.
There were two levels of condition for the RCT (compatible and incom-
patible) and three levels for the SCT (direction, location, and conflict). We
expected to find a main effect of condition, such that children, irrespective
of group, would make more errors and exhibit longer RTs on the incom-
patible/conflict conditions relative to the compatible conditions; these
findings would support the validity of the task manipulations. It was also
predicted that at-risk preschoolers would display circumscribed deficits in
motor inhibition (RCT) and interference control (SCT) relative to controls,
by performing disproportionately worse on the incompatible conditions
compared with the control conditions (Group
.18 (see
Figure 1B). At-risk participants did show a tendency to respond
more rapidly irrespective of condition; however, this finding did
not reach statistical significance: group main effect, F (1,
59) 3.55, p .06, 2
.06. Finally, no significant Group
Condition interaction was identified, F (1, 59) 1.01, p .10, 2
.02.
SCT
Condition interactions).
For the DNMST and RMT, the number of correct responses served as
dependent measures. As described above, group-specific deficits in the
domains assessed by the two tasks (i.e., nonverbal working memory and
memory for relative time, respectively) would require a significant
Group
.31 (see Figure 2A). Post hoc comparisons revealed that both
groups committed significantly more errors on Condition 3 relative
to Conditions 1 and 2. There was no significant main effect for
group, F (1, 64) 2.59, p .10, 2
Condition interaction, with at-risk preschoolers performing dif-
ferentially worse than controls on the experimental condition.
Objectives 2 and 3: Relationship of neuropsychological status to labo-
ratory measures of activity level and parent and teacher ratings of ADHD
symptoms. Linear regression procedures were used to remove the effects
of age on waist and ankle actigraph counts and to generate standardized
residual values. Performance discrepancies between the experimental and
control conditions were then calculated for each neuropsychological task.
For the RCT and SCT, the total number of errors on the control condition
was subtracted from the number of errors committed on the experimental
condition. For the DNMST and RMT, the number of correct responses on
the experimental condition was subtracted from the total number of correct
responses on the control condition. Finally, two-tailed Bonferroni-cor-
rected Pearson product–moment correlations were calculated to examine
the associations between each difference score and actigraph counts ob-
tained from the waist and ankle. Median movement counts were used in all
actigraph analyses to minimize the impact of outliers. Similarly, correla-
tions between the task difference scores and parent and teacher ratings of
hyperactivity–impulsivity (dimensional summary scores) were examined.
.04, nor was there a
significant Group Condition interaction, F (2, 63) 1.02, p
.10, 2
.03.
Using RT as the dependent measure yielded a significant main
effect for condition, F (2, 63) 18.47, p .001, 2
.37 (see
Figure 2B), with both groups exhibiting longer RTs on Condition 3
compared with Conditions 1 and 2. At-risk participants also re-
sponded more rapidly than controls across all three task condi-
tions: group main effect, F (1, 64) 4.42, p .04, 2
.07.
However, group differences in RT did not systematically vary as a
function of SCT condition: Group Condition interaction, F (2,
63) 2.50, p .09, 2
.07.
DNMST
As was the case for both the RCT and SCT, a main effect for
condition was found, with participants performing significantly
worse on the experimental condition relative to the control condi-
tion, F (1, 52) 138.20, p .001, 2
.78 (see Figure 3).
However, there was no significant main effect for group, F (1,
52) 0.11, p .10, 2
Results
.002, nor was there a significant
Group Condition interaction, F (1, 52) 2.26, p .10,
2
Although the nature of the DNMST and the RMT precluded
evaluations of the tasks’ reliability, split-half reliability estimates
were calculated using error rates and RT variables from the dif-
ferent conditions of the RCT and SCT. Split-half reliabilities for
RCT error rates and RTs ranged from .71 to .77 and from .51 to
.61, respectively. Reliability estimates for error rates and RTs on
the SCT ranged from .78 to .84 and from .45 to .77, respectively.
.04.
RMT
As shown in Figure 4, a significant main effect for condition
again supported the validity of the task manipulation, with children
In support of task validity, a significant main effect for condition
was observed for total errors, F (2, 63) 14.41, p .001, 2
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin