Gilles Fauconier - Cognitive linguistics.pdf

(67 KB) Pobierz
Microsoft Word - Cognitive ling
Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science
Cognitive Linguistics
Gilles Fauconnier
Cognitive linguistics has emerged in the last twenty-five years as a
powerful approach to the study of language, conceptual systems, human
cognition, and general meaning construction.
It addresses within language the structuring of basic conceptual categories
such as space and time, scenes and events, entities and processes, motion and
location, force and causation. It addresses the structuring of ideational and
affective categories attributed to cognitive agents, such as attention and
perspective, volition and intention. 1 In doing so, it develops a rich conception of
grammar that reflects fundamental cognitive abilities: the ability to form
structured conceptualizations with multiple levels of organization, to conceive of
a situation at varying levels of abstraction, to establish correspondences between
facets of different structures, and to construe the same situation in alternate
ways. 2
Cognitive linguistics recognizes that the study of language is the study of
language use and that when we engage in any language activity, we draw
unconsciously on vast cognitive and cultural resources, call up models and
frames, set up multiple connections, coordinate large arrays of information, and
engage in creative mappings, transfers, and elaborations. Language does not
2
"represent" meaning; it prompts for the construction of meaning in particular
contexts with particular cultural models and cognitive resources. Very sparse
grammar guides us along the same rich mental paths, by prompting us to
perform complex cognitive operations. Thus, a large part of cognitive linguistics
centers on the creative on-line construction of meaning as discourse unfolds in
context. 3 The dividing line between semantics and pragmatics dissolves and
truth-conditional compositionality disappears.
Aspects of language and expression that had been consigned to the
rhetorical periphery of language, such as metaphor 4 and metonymy, 5 are
redeemed and rehabilitated within cognitive linguistics. They are understood to
be powerful conceptual mappings at the very core of human thought, important
not just for the understanding of poetry, but also science, mathematics, religion,
philosophy, and everyday speaking and thinking. 6
Importantly, thought and language are embodied. Conceptual structure
arises from our sensorimotor experience and the neural structures that give rise
to it. The structure of concepts includes prototypes; reason is embodied and
imaginative. A grammar is ultimately a neural system. The properties of
grammars are the properties of humanly embodied neural systems. 7 Cognitive
capacities that play a fundamental role in the organization of language are not
specific to language. Such capacities include analogy, recursion, viewpoint and
perspective, figure-ground organization, and conceptual integration. 8
3
The stage was set for cognitive linguistics in the nineteen seventies and
early eighties with Len Talmy's work on figure and ground, Ronald Langacker's
cognitive grammar framework, George Lakoff's research on metaphor, gestalts,
categories and prototypes, 9 Fillmore's frame semantics, 10 and Fauconnier's mental
spaces. 11 Today, there are hundreds of scholars who work in this paradigm, and
there is a huge amount of published research on the theories and their
applications. The present short encyclopedia entry cannot do justice to the
wealth of discoveries, empirical studies, and applications that have come out of
this recent tradition. I give in the bibliography some indications of where to get
a more detailed picture. 12 In the following sections, I outline some of the
fundamental themes that run through cognitive linguistics.
I. Grammar and Cognition
The relation of grammar to cognition is studied in exquisite detail in the
foundational work of Talmy (2000) and Langacker (1987, 1991). Talmy shows the
great restrictions on the conceptual categories that grammatical systems actually
specifiy. For example number, but not color, and within number, 'singular',
'dual', 'plural', but never 'even', 'odd', 'dozen' or 'numerable'. Topological
reference ('across the sky,' 'across the table,'), but not Euclidean reference.
Multiplexing, states of boundedness and dividedness. Axiality, perspective ( The
door slowly opened and two men walked in vs. Two men slowly opened the door and
walked in ), sequentializing ( There are some houses in the valley vs. There is a house
now and then in the valley ), viewing, and nesting.
4
Langacker shows how grammar imposes trajector/landmark organization
on scenes and events ( The table is below the lamp and The lamp is above the table
express the same spatial relation with trajector and landmark reversed).
Profiling is another important construct of Langacker's cognitive grammar: the
word hypotenuse evokes a right triangle and profiles a particular part of it: the
same segment without the rest of the triangle is no longer a hypotenuse. In I
melted it , melt profiles an entire action chain with causation and change leading
to a liquid state. In It melted easily , only the change is profiled, although the
causation is still evoked. In It is finally melted , only the resultant state is profiled,
but the unprofiled change is evoked. Langacker analyzes in considerable detail
the ways in which component structures are integrated through correspondences
and elaboration to form composite structures: a phonological integration (e.g.
the English jar lid ) symbolizes the semantic integration of 'jar' and 'lid'. 13
Other key aspects of conceptual structuring as reflected by grammar, and
found in language after language, 14 include fictive motion ( The blackboard goes all
the way to the wall ), event integration ( The ball rolled in , The candle blew out , I kicked
the door shut ), and force dynamics ( The ball kept rolling , He refrained from closing the
door ), and the application of force dynamics to abstract reasoning and felicity
conditions on speech acts. 15
Fascinating linguistically and psychologically is the way in which
language structures space. No two languages are ever alike in this respect,
although the general principles remain the same. Each of us in his or her own
5
language carves out physical space in fantastically intricate ways, of which we
are not aware. Deceptively simple looking prepositions like in , out , over, define
elaborate networks of spatial meaning with hundreds of linked schemas, some of
which are prototypical and central. Compare The plane flew over the field , The
post-office is over the hill , The log rolled over , The party is over , He had to do it over , He
overlooked it, He looked it over, He oversaw it . Remarkable work on this topic has
been done by cognitive linguists 16 and explicit computational models 17 have
confirmed the staggering cognitive complexity of the human capacity to
structure space linguistically. Regier (1996), who built a structured connectionist
model capable of learning subsets of such relations for different languages shows
that many aspects of neurobiology, conceptualization, and general learning are at
play.
II. Metaphor theory
A second strand of fundamental work in cognitive linguistics which
interacts constantly with the first is the considerable development of metaphor
theory over the last twenty years. Launched by Lakoff and Johnson (1980), this
line of research rests on the key insight that far from being a rhetorical flourish,
metaphor is basic and constitutive for all the thinking that we do, and that in the
scheme of evolution, metaphor, based on source domains of human experience
and neural connections to our embodied sensations, actions, and emotions, is
what creates the possibility of 'abstract' reasoning, scientific and mathematical
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin